|
|
Line 63: |
Line 63: |
| perception. Journal of Vision, 9, 1 -12. </b><br>[<a | | perception. Journal of Vision, 9, 1 -12. </b><br>[<a |
| | | |
- | href="http://journalofvision.org/9/2/8/" target="_blank">Related articles</a>]<br><br> | + | href="http://journalofvision.org/9/2/8/" target="_blank">Related articles</a>]<br> |
| + | This paper describes a novel psychophysical and analytical technique, called periodic perturbation, for creating and characterizing perceptual waves associated with transitions in visibility of a stimulus during binocular rivalry and during binocular fusion. Observers tracked rivalry within a small, central region of spatially extended rival targets while small, brief increments in contrast (“triggers”) were presented repetitively in antiphase within different regions of the two rival targets. Appropriately timed triggers produced entrainment of rivalry alternations within the central region, with the optimal timing dependent on an observer's native alternation rate. The latency between trigger and state switch increased with the distance between the location of the trigger and the central region being monitored, providing evidence for traveling waves of dominance. Traveling waves produced by periodic perturbation exhibited the same characteristics as those generated using a less efficient, more demanding discrete trial technique. We used periodic perturbation to reveal a novel relation between the dynamics associated with the spontaneous perceptual alternations and the speed of traveling waves across observers. In addition, we found evidence for traveling waves even when the events triggering them were initiated within regions of the visual field where binocular vision was stable, in the absence of binocular rivalry, implying that perceptual organization generally depends on spatio-temporal context. <br><br> |
| | | |
| | | |
Line 72: |
Line 73: |
| href="http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi? | | href="http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi? |
| | | |
- | artid=2720072" target="_blank">PubMed</a>]<br><br> | + | artid=2720072" target="_blank">PubMed</a>]<br> |
| + | When the two eyes view dissimilar monocular stimuli, the resulting interocular suppression can spread beyond the region of explicit stimulus conflict: portions of one rival target will disappear even though there is no competing stimulation at the corresponding location in the other eye’s view. In a series of experiments we examined whether this spread of suppression is spatially isotropic or governed by the configuration of the stimulus a portion of which is subject to suppression. Observers reported the incidence of stimulus disappearance at different locations along or nearby the contours of a large figure, part of which was suppressed by presentation of a continuous flash-suppression stimulus to a restricted region of the other eye. For all observers, suppression spread over several degrees along the contours of the figure, but tended not to spread to locations nearby but disconnected from the figure. Suppression spread effectively over a smoothly curved contour, and it spread around a sharp corner defined by two abutting contours, albeit less effectively. Suppression tended not to spread to features within the interior of a figure (a face), even if those features formed an integral part of the figure. A gap within a spatially extended stimulus arrested the spread of suppression, unless that gap appeared to arise from occlusion. Spread of suppression was unrelated to sensory eye dominance and was found with a more conventional binocular rivalry configuration, too. These findings implicate the involvement of neural circuitry in which inhibition propagates along paths of excitation beyond spatial regions of explicit interocular conflict. |
| + | <br><br> |
| </html> | | </html> |
| | | |
Revision as of 05:49, 24 June 2010
System and Cognitive unit
Sang-Hun Lee, PhD, Dept. of Psychology, SNU
HP
2009

*
Asymmetric interaction between motion and stereopsis
23 revealed by concurrent adaptation.
[
Article (PDF)]
Although contingent aftereffects between motion and stereopsis have been referred to as behavioral evidence for the joint
processing of the two features, the reciprocal nature of encoding the two features has not been systematically studied. Using a novel form of concurrent adaptation, we probed the perception of direction- and disparity-de!ned coherent surfaces
in parallel before and after adaptation to a stimulus that moved in a single direction at a particular binocular disparity.
Contrary to earlier !ndings, we found a strong asymmetry between motion and stereopsis: the detection of disparity signal
after adaptation was more impaired when the test stimulus was moving in the adapted direction than in the non-adapted
direction, whereas the test disparity hardly affected the detection of coherent motion. However, motion adaptation became
dependent on disparity when we added another surface that was moving in the opposite direction at the opposite sign of
disparity to those of the original adaptor, as in previous studies of contingent aftereffects. The observed asymmetric
contingency between motion and disparity adaptation urges the reinterpretation of previously reported contingent aftereffects
and suggests a corresponding asymmetry between neural mechanisms devoted to processing of motion and stereopsis in
human visual cortex.
2007
*
Hierarchy of cortical responses underlying
binocular rivalry. [
>Article (PDF)]
During binocular rivalry, physical stimulation is dissociated from conscious visual awareness. Human brain imaging reveals a tight
linkage between the neural events in human primary visual cortex (V1) and the dynamics of perceptual waves during transitions in
dominance during binocular rivalry. Here, we report results from experiments in which observers’ attention was diverted from the
rival stimuli, implying that: competition between two rival stimuli involves neural circuits in V1, and attention is crucial for the
consequences of this neural competition to advance to higher visual areas and promote perceptual waves.
2005
*
Traveling waves of activity in primary visual cortex during binocular rivalry. [
Article (PDF)]
When the two eyes view large, dissimilar patterns that induce binocular rivalry, alternating waves of visibility arre experienced as one pattern sweeps the other out of conscious awaerness. Here we combine psychophysics with functional magnetic resonance imaging to show tight linkage between dynamics of perceptual waves during ribalry and neural events in human primary wisual cortex (V1).
Randolph Blake, PhD, Dept. of Psychology, Vanderbilt University
HP
2010
[
Publication list]
2009
*
Periodic perturbations producing phase-locked fluctuations in visual
perception. Journal of Vision, 9, 1 -12. [
Related articles]
This paper describes a novel psychophysical and analytical technique, called periodic perturbation, for creating and characterizing perceptual waves associated with transitions in visibility of a stimulus during binocular rivalry and during binocular fusion. Observers tracked rivalry within a small, central region of spatially extended rival targets while small, brief increments in contrast (“triggers”) were presented repetitively in antiphase within different regions of the two rival targets. Appropriately timed triggers produced entrainment of rivalry alternations within the central region, with the optimal timing dependent on an observer's native alternation rate. The latency between trigger and state switch increased with the distance between the location of the trigger and the central region being monitored, providing evidence for traveling waves of dominance. Traveling waves produced by periodic perturbation exhibited the same characteristics as those generated using a less efficient, more demanding discrete trial technique. We used periodic perturbation to reveal a novel relation between the dynamics associated with the spontaneous perceptual alternations and the speed of traveling waves across observers. In addition, we found evidence for traveling waves even when the events triggering them were initiated within regions of the visual field where binocular vision was stable, in the absence of binocular rivalry, implying that perceptual organization generally depends on spatio-temporal context.
*
Spatial spread of interocular suppression is guided by stimulus
configuration. Perception, 38, 215-231. [
PubMed]
When the two eyes view dissimilar monocular stimuli, the resulting interocular suppression can spread beyond the region of explicit stimulus conflict: portions of one rival target will disappear even though there is no competing stimulation at the corresponding location in the other eye’s view. In a series of experiments we examined whether this spread of suppression is spatially isotropic or governed by the configuration of the stimulus a portion of which is subject to suppression. Observers reported the incidence of stimulus disappearance at different locations along or nearby the contours of a large figure, part of which was suppressed by presentation of a continuous flash-suppression stimulus to a restricted region of the other eye. For all observers, suppression spread over several degrees along the contours of the figure, but tended not to spread to locations nearby but disconnected from the figure. Suppression spread effectively over a smoothly curved contour, and it spread around a sharp corner defined by two abutting contours, albeit less effectively. Suppression tended not to spread to features within the interior of a figure (a face), even if those features formed an integral part of the figure. A gap within a spatially extended stimulus arrested the spread of suppression, unless that gap appeared to arise from occlusion. Spread of suppression was unrelated to sensory eye dominance and was found with a more conventional binocular rivalry configuration, too. These findings implicate the involvement of neural circuitry in which inhibition propagates along paths of excitation beyond spatial regions of explicit interocular conflict.
Inah Lee, PhD, Dept. of Psychology, U of Iowa
HP
2010

*
Hippocampus is necessary for spatial discrimination using distal
cue-configuration. Hippocampus.
[
Article (PDF)]
*
Dentate gyrus is necessary for disambiguating similar object-place
representations.
[
Related articles] [
PDF]
*
Perirhinal cortex is necessary for acquiring, but not for retrieving,
object-place paired association.
[
Related articles] [
PDF]
Marcus Kaiser, PhD, Complex Neural Systems, Newcastle Univ.
HP
2010
*
Optimal hierarchical modular topologies for producing limited
sustained activation of neural networks.
[
Article (PDF)]
*
A nonsynaptic mechanism underlying interictal discharges in
human epileptic neocortex. [
Article (PDF)]
*
Reducing influenza spreading over the airline network.
[
Article (PDF)]
*
Parallel calculation of multi-electrode array correlation networks.
[
Article (PDF)]
*
Random outgrowth and spatial competition generate realistic
connection length distributions and filling fractions.[
Article (PDF)]
*
Beyond the average: detecting global singular nodes from local
features in complex networks. [
Article (PDF)]